Critically Evaluate the Contribution of Experiments in helping to Understand what goes on in Groups

Authors Avatar

R0749273

Critically Evaluate the Contribution of Experiments in Helping Us To Understand What Goes On In Groups

Brown (1996, p44) provides a utilitarian definition of groups which goes to the heart of the experimental perspective on groups: “We define an aggregate of people as a group if we can discern two aspects. First, that individuals think of themselves as group members who experience a sense of belongingness and a common sense of identity. Second, that participants have psychological effects on each other which includes affective, cognitive and behavioural aspects”. This definition sets up a position which highlights the focus of experimental work on groups and is indicative of both the strengths and weaknesses of this approach. It is to be expected that the approach is deterministic, with an emphasis on cause and effect. However, the definition is suggestive of a concern that is broader than simple behavioural outcomes, with emotional and cognitive processes being considered. It is clear that groups are perceived as aggregates of individuals and this marks a departure from other approaches where the group is perceived as a de facto entity. But, the requirement for a ‘sense of belongingness’ does mean that group-level processes cannot be assumed to exist in any particular collection of people or demographic stratum.

In this essay I will set out some of the positive contributions that the experimental approach to groups has provided. Group experiments offer some interesting insights into group processes in two major areas: conflict between groups and the effects of group membership on individuals. The second of these is the primary focus here, and as such I will consider the experimental evidence of the influence of group membership on individual members. This will cover the making of decisions in the presence of others; conformity with the expectations of a perceived peer group; obeying/resisting authority; the capacity of individuals to influence the group; and the effect of identification on self-esteem. Since it is important to locate and recognise the limitations of the experimental approach, I will provide caveats to the evidence given throughout, and subsequently consider whether the insights gleaned are at a true group-level. In doing this I will compare the experimental approach with the ‘group psychodynamic paradigm’.

Dealing first with conformity, there are two classic studies in this field. Sherif (1936, cited by Brown 1996, p14) used an optical illusion, the ‘autokinetic effect’ of a stationary point of light, to measure how individual estimates of the movement of the light differed when given alone and in the presence of other participants. Sherif found that, on their own, individuals developed there own norms and ranges of movement which varied widely, but on subsequent grouping in twos and threes estimates became similar but without averaging. The individual frame of reference was lost when presented with those of others. In the reverse case, the frame of reference from the group situation persisted when the individuals were subsequently tested alone.

Join now!

Whilst this experiment is indicative of some tendency to influence and the persistence of that influence, the ambiguous stimulus prevents solid inference about the strength of effect being made. Asch (1952, cited by Brown 1996, pp. 15-17) used an unambiguous stimulus “[T]o study the personal and social conditions that induce individuals to resist or to yield to group pressures when the latter are perceived to be contrary to fact”.

The experiment presented one naïve subject with a decision about line lengths. Seven experimenter confederates gave their unanimous erroneous judgement publicly, thus presenting the subject with evidence contrary to ...

This is a preview of the whole essay