"The Barnum Effect" states that individuals are likely to accept general personality descriptions, this study aims to test this theory and further the research

Authors Avatar

Hannah Baumer

The difference in acceptance of personality descriptions when known to be randomly selected or thought to be personal horoscopes

Abstract

“The Barnum Effect” states that individuals are likely to accept general personality descriptions, this study aims to test this theory and further the research by testing whether specificity has an effect on the accuracy rating. Participants were split into two groups: the general group and horoscope group, all were given a general personality description, the horoscope group were told these corresponded to their astrological signs and the general group were made aware this was a general description. Both groups rated the descriptions as fairly accurate, supporting “The Barnum Effect” as predicted. Using an independent t-test we analysed the results and found there was no significant difference between the two groups’ ratings and therefore no effect was found through varying the specificity. However this finding may be due to only having two groups, an improvement of the study would be to include more levels of specificity.

Introduction

Personality is an aspect of psychology that forms a key research area, the way in which individuals' thoughts, feelings, and behaviours differ, and the stability of these differences over time is of great interest to psychologists (Pervin & John, 2001). Reliable and valid personality measures have been developed as a requirement to further studies into underlying personality characteristics and the variables that mediate them. One such type of personality assessment is multiple-choice or true/false judgements within tests, allowing the items to be scored objectively (Anastase & Urbina, 1997). An example of such a test would be to present a respondent with a series of statements about their personality and ask them to rate their accuracy.

Early studies examining the acceptance of personality descriptions showed a tendency amongst individuals to believe generalised and ambiguous statements as being true (Forer, 1949). Regardless of the fact that these bogus descriptions could apply to most people, respondents tend to believe them to be uniquely applicable to themselves, this is known as the Barnum Effect, as labelled by Paul Meehl (1956). Forer's (1949) original study involved giving participants a personality test, followed by a personality evaluation, which they were informed would be based on their earlier personality test. All participants then received the same evaluation. Despite this, participants rated the accuracy of the evaluation and a mean of "good" to "excellent" was found. In general, all the acceptance tests have approached the method in a similar way, following the same four steps (a) completing a personality test; (b) waiting while the test is scored; (c) receiving the interpretation; (d) rating the accuracy of the interpretation. Most participants of such tests will receive the same general "Barnum" interpretation (Snyder et al. 1975). There are a number of studies that have since replicated this effect. Furnham (1994) presented participants with vague descriptions about their health, despite being bogus statements, they were again rated as accurate by participants. However not all psychologists believe that the Barnum results hold validity as they cannot be applied to individuals outside of the sample used, as the college students who were chosen as subjects are relatively naive and "unsophisticated" (Dana and Graham 1976). There is further evidence to suggest that acceptance may vary with the "sophistication" of the population, as psychiatric residents were less likely to accept the general personality descriptions than the younger undergraduate and graduate students (Bachrach and Pattishell 1960). The number of residents was so small however, that this finding may lack the validity to be used as a criticism against the Barnum results. In support of the Barnum results, Forer (1949) found no relationship between acceptance and either age or occupational background, and Stagner (1958) found Barnum results to have an equally high level of acceptance from college students, industrial supervisors and personnel managers alike. The Barnum effect may offer at least a partial explanation as to why people continue to believe in astrology (Fichten & Sunerton, 1983).  Piper-Terry & Downey (1998) offered women’s’ helpfulness as an explanation as to why female participants concur with their friends’ bogus test interpretations, however Layne argues against this point stating that it women are more open and thus expect their friend’s interpretations to be more accurate. Gullibility of women is also an aspect which psychologists have looked into to explain the Barnum effect, in contradiction to any research into the Barnum effect based on gender Forer (1949), Houston (1976), Snyder & Shenkel (1976) and many others have all reported no differences in the acceptance of personality descriptions by males and females. From this array of supporting research it would appear that both sexes are equally susceptible to the Barnum effect.

Join now!

        Snyder (1974) identified the relevance and specificity of the interpretation to be a further factor in enhancing the Barnum effect. In this study, three groups were presented with the same personality descriptions. One group were told this was a general description, the second group were told the profile was produced using astrological charts from the year and month which they were born, the final group were told the descriptions were based on knowledge of the year, month, and day they were born. Those in the general group rated their descriptions as least accurate, with those in the most specific group ...

This is a preview of the whole essay