The Female Orgasm: Adaptation, Artefact or culturally learned? ( Department of Psychology - University of Liverpool)

Authors Avatar

The Human Female Orgasm: Adaptation, Artefact, or culturally learned?                Rajat Passy

Biological Explanations of Human Behaviour                Department of Psychology

                        Liverpool Hope University

The Human Female Orgasm:

Adaptation, Artefact, or culturally learned?

Rajat Passy

Department of Psychology

University of Liverpool

Liverpool

United Kingdom


The Human Female Orgasm: Adaptation, Artefact, or culturally learned?

Word Count: 2958 (excluding in-text and end-text referencing and quotations)

The female orgasm has been a source of fascination for a variety of groups from sex researchers to the lay public, and evolutionary psychologists are no exception. Evolutionists have not had much difficulty explaining the male orgasm, due to its direct relationship with reproduction. But the Darwinian rationale behind the female orgasm has remained elusive. Women are able to have sexual intercourse and even conceive a child – doing their part for the perpetuation of the species – without experiencing orgasm (Smith NY times, 2005). Unlike the unicorn, (which is specially interesting precisely because it does not exist) or extrasensory perception (which probably does not exist but is interesting because of the possibility that it might) or the above mentioned male orgasm (which exists with monotonous regularity and for the most part is interesting only to people directly involved in one) the female orgasm definitely exists and yet arouses interest, debate, ideology, technical manuals and popular literature simply because it is so often absent (Symons, 1979). There have been numerous evolutionary accounts put forward by researchers in the last 70 years attempting to explain the female orgasm but very few have gained academic credence.

The first section of this paper will consider two prevailing evolutionary arguments for its existence i.e. the pair-bond account (Morris, 1967) and the intermittent reinforcement theory (Diamond, 1980 and Hrdy, 1979) and argue that they suffer from one or more debilitating flaws either in the way they interpret evidence or how their argument is constructed. While evaluating the above accounts, it will also reveal two main biases which have adversely affected the evolutionary explanations of the female orgasm. One of the biases is assuming that the female orgasm evolved to its current condition because it somehow facilitated the reproductive success of its possessor (Lloyd, 2005). The second bias is assuming that female sexuality is like male sexuality; which says men and women have similar sexual responses. After presenting overwhelming evidence of women responding differently to intercourse than men, it will progress to the second section with a theory that assumes neither an adaptive function of the orgasm nor an androcentric account of it (Lloyd, 2005) but explains the phenomenon with the best supporting research available today. The final section will present a critique of biological accounts of the female orgasm and describe cultural (psychological, demographic and early experiences) influences shaping definitions, ideas and experiences of the female orgasm.

The first explanation revolves around the notion of a pair bond – an enduring monogamous partnership between males and females (Crook, 1972). The majority of adaptive explanations of the human female orgasm are based on the idea that a male-female pair bond is adaptive and that female orgasm helped with pair bonding. Possibly the most developed theory is that of Desmond Morris’s (1967) which will briefly be presented and critically evaluated. Morris (1967) attempts to determine that pair bonding would itself be a significant evolutionary adaptation for our pre-hominid and hominid ancestors. Females would develop a tendency to pair with males because “males had to be sure that their females were going to be faithful to them when they left them alone to go hunting” (1967, p. 64). After establishing the evolutionary significance of the pair-bond Morris provides an unexpected explanation of the role of intercourse. Intercourse is meant to be a way to maintain the pair bond and the greater part of our species is evidently concerned not with producing offspring, but with strengthening the pair-bond by having mutual rewards for each sexual partner (Morris, 1967). Such rewards may include orgasms for the female. Thus, because pair bonds are adaptive for maintaining the social structure of hunting, virtually anything that would help support the pair bond is also adaptive, including the female orgasm (Lloyd, 2005). However, this account only works if females were significantly less able to achieve orgasm outside the pair bond or vice versa.

Morris introduces his explanation of the female orgasm by summarizing his version of the human sexual response. He appears to generalize from the male sexual response to the female response while overlooking the information available on female sexuality. Although he is aware that during intercourse the female orgasm does not occur as frequently as that of the males, he writes: “If the male continues to copulate for a longer period of time, the female also eventually reaches a consummatory moment … on the average it is attained between ten and twenty minutes after the start of copulation” (1967, p. 54). He continues to express surprise at the discrepancy between the male and female times to reach sexual climax.

One flaw of this account is that, according to previous research, it turns out that females take no longer than males to reach orgasm; that only happens during intercourse (Lloyd, 2005). Kinsey and others found that the time to orgasm in women (during masturbation) is the same as for men (Kinsey et al. 1953). Morris continues to say that after both partners have experienced orgasm through intercourse, a period of exhaustion, relaxation and frequently sleep follows right after (Morris, 1967). He asserts that after climax has been reached, all the previous physiological changes that orgasm brought about are rapidly reversed and the post-sexual individual returns to a dormant inactive physiological state. On the contrary, although the tendency to sleep and exhaustion is true for men, it is not true for women (Kaplan, 1974). Concerning the claim that the physiological changes are “rapidly reversed,” that is also only true for men, but not women who actually reach a phase of excitement after orgasm, rather than the original unexcited phase (Masters and Johnson, 1966). This is also supported by the experiences of the women in Hite’s study, where the majority of women did not indicate a return to the base state (Hite, 1976). The women reported two basic kinds of feelings – wanting to be close and “feeling strong and wide awake, energetic and alive.” Hite interpreted these reactions indicating a continued feeling of arousal. Shope (1967 cited in Lloyd, 2005) reported that 25% of women who had orgasm with coitus described increased states of energy after coitus during daylight hours. Morris’s disregard of this important disparity between male and female sexuality testifies against part of his own adaptive explanation.

Join now!

Although he offers a detailed technical description of female sexual response during intercourse his account includes some perplexing features. He notes that the extra thickness of the human male penis is designed to help stimulate the woman; and results in the female’s external genitals going through much more pulling and pushing during pelvic thrusts. This along with pressure from the pubic region of the male would translate to virtual masturbation - if she were a male. According to Lloyd (2005), Morris is evidently using Masters and Johnson’s model of indirect clitoral stimulation caused by thrusting. As the penis moves ...

This is a preview of the whole essay