• Join over 1.2 million students every month
  • Accelerate your learning by 29%
  • Unlimited access from just £6.99 per month

Morphological structure variation - Italian, English, Swahili and Chinese the languages considered

Free essay example:

Practice essay:

Explain, with reference to appropriate examples, how morphological structure can vary between languages.

  1. Introduction

According to Bauer (1988 : 248), morphology is “the study of the ways in which lexemes and word-forms are built up from smaller elements, and the changes that are made to those smaller elements in the process of building lexemes and word-forms”. In morphology, the words of a language are classified into nouns, verbs, adjectives, and a few other classes.

In this paper, I will concentrate more on the structure and on the properties of nouns and verbs in order to analyse, depending on the type of language, how morphological structure vary between languages.

I will also take a look, regarding nouns, to morphological variation, and, regarding verbs, to verbal inflection.

But before doing so, I must say that languages are classified on the basis of their morphological characteristics, even though there can always be exceptions [overlappings]: hence, we have isolating, agglutinative and fusional languages. Chinese, Swahili and Italian belong respectively to each one of these categories, while English represents a “particular case” in its morphological characteristics.

Languages as Chinese do not have a morphology, because there are no changes in the internal form of a Chinese word: thus, there is no distinction between a word and a morpheme.

The case of agglutinative languages, instead, is the opposite: in Swahili, one of the most widely spread languages of Africa, for example, a single word is characterized by more morphemes, which can be easily separated.

Italian, as well as other Indo-European languages, belongs to the category of fusional languages, where a single morpheme can express more than only one meaning and where there are plenty of examples of allomorphs, (two or more complementary morphs which manifest a morpheme in its different phonological or morphological environments).

  1. Nouns

According to Gibbon, English nouns are composed by a stem and an inflection.

A stem, like “table” or “hand”, has a lexical meaning, and relates a word to its syntactic (through subject-verb agreement) and semantic context. It can be simple or complex, like “beautiful” (stem + derivational affix), “armchair” (stem + stem), “bus-driver” (stem + stem + derivation).

Inflexions of English words are suffixes, like the “–s” of the plural, stem vowel changes, as in “foot-feet” and prefixes, as in “unhappy”.

In Italian, as Ranta points out, nouns have the parameter of number. Each noun has two forms, the singular and the plural. Italian has a small number of noun declensions: if a noun ends with “-e”, as piede (foot), its plural form will end with “-i”, piedi (feet); if a noun ends with “–a”, as testa (head), its plural form will end with “–e”, teste (heads); finally, if a noun ends with “-o”, as testo (text), its plural form will end with “-i,” testi (texts).

In Swahili, as Myachina (1981 : 22) argues, nouns are divided into classes by logical correlation: thus we have the class of objects, the class of human beings, the class of animals, the class of trees; a class can be also determined by mutual relationship of two objects in space. Swahili has altogether 15 classes. Each class has a different set of prefixes for marking numbers (singular and plural), e.g., mtoto "child", watoto "children".

Regarding Chinese language, Packard (2000 : 69) believes that, depending on the free-bound position of a morpheme and on its content-grammatical property, four types of morphemes can be distinguished: function words(grammatical and free morphemes) as “hé” 合 (Conj.), root words (content and free morphemes), as ng(ice), bound words (content and bound morphemes), as diànnǎo 电脑, electic-brain (computer) and affixes (grammatical and bound morphemes).

It must be said that only root words and bound words may be stems and word heads, while only root words may stand alone as free words. In the category of affixes, we can distinguish between word-forming affixes (which may be heads as well), as “wú-” 无 (not) and grammatical affixes, as “-men” , the plural affix.

Morphophonemic alternation

        Languages which are characterised by morphophonemic alternation show variations in the phonemic and/or morphological shape of their morphemes, showing the presence of allomorphs.

For example, both English and Italian have this characteristic: nevertheless, while in English examples of this type are less common, in Italian are much more common. In English examples are the plurals “-es” and “-s” as in buses /iz/ or girls /z/; the plural of “-f”, which is “-ves”, as in “wives”; in Italian many examples can be taken from nouns like psicologo /psi’kolo:go/ (psychologist), which assumes different shapes depending on the context. Thus, we find psicologi /psi’colodЗI/ (psychologists), psicologico /psiko’lodЗi:ko/ (psychologic), or psicologia /psikolo’Ia/ (psychology), where the lexical morpheme /g/ becomes /dЗ/. Another example can be also taken from the adjective b[‘ε]llo (beautiful) and b[e]llino (fairly beautiful), where the difference between the two vowels is justified by phonological reasons.

Morphophonemic alternation in Swahili is due mainly to palatalisation and labialisation. Palatalisation of consonants arises where the high front vowel i is present. Thus, in a variety of Swahili suffixes we can find the shift n > m before labials, e.g. n + -buzi > mbuzi; or the shift k > ch /t∫/ before i + a, as in ki + angu > changu.

In Chinese, it is easy to deduct that there is no morphophonemic alternation, since every morpheme is isolated and there is no grammatical agreement.

However, this does not mean that in Chinese grammatical values and functions do not exist in Mandarin: as Bauer (1988 : 131) points out, there is a set of grammatical morphemes in Mandarin with serves to mark nouns with grammatical cases, as bèi 被for the nominative or agentive.

  1. Verbs

Verbal inflection

English verbal inflection is not that rich: in fact, for almost all verbs, there is only one change, adding -s or -es for third person singular, only in the Present Tense: “He listens”. All other persons require no changes to the verb: “I dream”, “You dream”, “We dream”, and “They dream”.

In Italian, the situation is rather different: verbs are inflected in mode, tense, number, and person, so we have different forms for each person. An example may be the Indicative present tense of the verb mangiare (to eat): io mangio (I eat), tu mangi (you eat), egli mangia (he eats), noi mangiamo (we eat), voi mangiate (you eat), essi mangiano (they eat).

Besides, as an example of fusional language, a single Italian morpheme can encode various meanings: take a look at the 3rd person singular Past Tense of the verb amare (to love), amò: this verb encodes the 3rd person, the singular, the Past, the Indicative and the Active form at the same time.

Regarding Swahili verbal system, Myachina (1981 : 46) argues that the Swahili verb consists of more elements, as: a significant verbal base; preverbal concords in agreement with the class of the subject and the object; and tense, aspect and modal markers. An example can be mwanafunzi anasoma kitabu (the pupil reads the book), where “a-” is the subject concord of the 3rd person singular, agreeing with the class 1 noun mwanafunzi, “-na-” is the Present Tense marker, and “soma” (to read) is the significant verbal base. In Swahili, every morpheme can be easy separated: an agglutinative language is easily recognizable by this characteristic.

In isolating languages as Chinese, word order and particles shape the syntax and the meaning of the words, what inflection does in agglutinative and fusional languages.

Chinese verbal system, in fact, lacks verbal inflection, since its verbs do not change form, relying on word order, context or some kinds of morphemes to convey meaning: I will go is simply literally translated into “tomorrow (it depends on the speaker) I go”,ming tian wo jiang hui qu 明天我将会去 or “I bought a bookis translated with mai le yi ben shu 买了一本书, where the morpheme le is the Past Tense marker.

In Chinese, it is possible to divide words into morphemes, while in English it can create ambiguity (Bauer : 130): for instance, the denotations of “know”in knowing a person, a proposition, and a language are sufficiently different that they can be translated into three different verbs in other languages. This is the reason for which English cannot be classified as a fusional or an isolating language, as it shares sometimes some properties with the former and sometimes with the latter.

  1. Conclusion

The knowledge we have about our native language is innate in everyone of us: therefore, find and give explanations is not that simple, and this is why many research and studies have been made in order to understand rules that make languages.

Furthermore, in this paper I did not only talk about my mother language, Italian, but also about other three languages, two of them I have never studied before (Chinese and Swahili).

Ideally, the task of a morphological analysis system would be to break the word down to its component morphemes and determine the meaning of the resulting decomposition.

From this work I understood that foreigners’ language acquisition can be compared to children' language acquisition, since both are starting to learn the rules which make a language and, therefore, they have expectations. However, while English children try to apply the rules of English, foreigners who may not know them, tend to apply the rules of their own language: hence, we can hear a Chinese speaker saying “tomorrow I go” or a person who speaks Swahili saying “I eat it the apple”, putting 2 objects referring to the same argument.


Bibliography

Bauer, Laurie (1988), Introducing Linguistic Morphology, Edinburgh : Edinburgh University Press.

Gibbon, Dafydd (2006-2007), Introduction to Linguistics: Morphology - word construction,B.A. British And American Studies. [Online] (URL http://wwwhomes.uni-bielefeld.de/~gibbon/Classes/Classes2006WS/IntroductionToLinguistics/05-IntroMorphology.pdf). Accessed 13th April 2007.

Myachina, E. N. (1981), The Swahili Language: A Descriptive Grammar, London : Routledge & Kegan Paul.

Packard, Jerome Lee (2000), The morphology of Chinese: A Linguistic And Cognitive Approach. Cambridge : Cambridge University Press.

Ranta, Aarne, 1+n Representations of Italian Morphology, [Online] (URL http://www.cs.chalmers.se/~aarne/tmp/contributions/ranta/italiano.html). Accessed 15th April 2007.

Houghton Mifflin Company (1996),[Online] The American Heritage, Book of English Usage (URL http://www.bartleby.com/64/C001/060.html). Accessed 15th April 2007.

This student written piece of work is one of many that can be found in our University Degree Creative Writing section.

(?)
Not the one? Search for your essay title...
  • Join over 1.2 million students every month
  • Accelerate your learning by 29%
  • Unlimited access from just £6.99 per month

Related University Degree Linguistics, Classics and related subjects Skills and Knowledge Essays

See our best essays

Related University Degree Creative Writing essays

  1. Case Study of Three Counselling Sessions.

    As I have stated about I believed I left the frame of reference when Val talked of the devastation of that night. The observer thought that I had left the frame of reference when I spoke about Val projecting her anger onto the police officer on the phone.

  2. Traditional Celebration of the Chinese New Year

    At this time, the family did everything possible to obtain a favorable report from the Kitchen God. On the evening of the 23rd, the family would give the Kitchen God a ritualistic farewell dinner with sweet foods and honey.

  1. corpus and concordance in comparing grammar in both speech and writing

    The most commonly element follows (play) in spoken corpus is It which occurred 12 times, but in written there was nothing. Task 3 (comparison of wider lexicogrammatical patterns) Play in infinitive form The most 5 frequent collocates: Word + Code Corpora 2-left 1- left 1- right 2-right Play&VVI Spoken 20

  2. Discuss the Power of Language and the value of language diversity.

    Some native scholars and elders believe that our language is our culture. Verna Kirkness, states in her book on Aboriginal Languages (1998), "The key to identity and retention of culture is one's ancestral language". But there is grave concern about the deterioration of languages worldwide.

  1. Rules of negation in British Sign Language

    It can also be shown by adding additional words to a sentence such as, 'not, nothing, never,' these are used to negate the sentence. They are known as negative lexical items, (NLI's) which can be used before or after the verb, to include negation, in a sentence.

  2. Placing a Biography in the Context of History

    from most Indian women, who are sponsored by their primary migrant husbands. However, similar to the few Indian women who did come alone, she came as a student in sciences.2 Unfortunately, Pooja found herself lacking resources with which to start a life in America.

  1. Marian is a 37 year old female who appears to be from a middle ...

    to him, well what do you want for dinner tonight, well I told you I've got a meeting on, oh yes, yes sorry I forgot (Co: hmm) whereas I didn't forget, I just, I had no idea (Co: yeah), absolutely no idea.

  2. A Sociolinguistic study. Is there such a thing as mens and womens language? ...

    Secondly, tag questions are used as a way to facilitate. Third, they often soften critical comments. Finally, they are used as confrontational and coercive devices (Holmes, 2008). According to Holme?s research (1984), it was mentioned that percentages of using tag questions between women and men are significantly different.

  • Over 160,000 pieces
    of student written work
  • Annotated by
    experienced teachers
  • Ideas and feedback to
    improve your own work