How useful are the calculations of heritabilities in understanding human behaviour? Historically, the study of heritability (defined as the proportion of phenotypic variance that can be accounted for by genetic variance)

Authors Avatar

Sara Serras Pereira
November 2005

Essay n° 3: How useful are the calculations of heritabilities in understanding human behaviour?

Historically, the study of heritability (defined as the proportion of phenotypic variance that can be accounted for by genetic variance) has been typically performed on twins, as it was begun by Galton in the nineteenth-century. The conclusions he draw from his observations on twins remained largely accepted by later studies. He wrote that ‘The twins who closely resembled each other in childhood and early youth, and were reared under not very dissimilar conditions, either grow unlike through the development of natural characteristics which had lain dormant at first, or else they continue their lives, keeping time like two watches, hardly to be thrown out of accord except by some physical jar. Nature is far stronger than Nurture within the limited range that I have been careful to assign to the latter’(Galton, 1875), and his assumption expressed in the last sentence has been confirmed by several studies, and some contemporary biologists and psychologists still defend it.

In this essay, to analyse the main trends of the heritability debate, I will focus mainly on intelligence and IQ data, for intelligence is a trait that has been intensively studied from the heritability perspective.

Twin studies are a classical method in heritability researches because they allow the investigator to compare pairs of individuals of the same age and family environment, but of different degrees of genetic relatedness, on some trait of interest. A greater resemblance for the identical twin pairs is taken as evidence that the trait in question is, to a certain extent, genetically determined.

The study of adoptive families is another important design: it examines the resemblance of genetically unrelated individuals growing up together in the same family, as parents and children or as siblings. If they are less similar, in respect to a particular trait, than corresponding members of biological families, the difference is taken as evidence of the role played by genes in determining that trait.

Adoption studies also permit direct inference on the part played by shared (a.k.a. ‘between families’) environment: if genetically unrelated family members show any resemblances at all, then those are evidence of the effect of the family environment. (if the possibility of selective placing of children by adoption agencies is excluded).
The role of shared environment can also be indirectly inferred by twin studies: if the twins are more similar than should be expected from the estimated genetic effect, the resemblance is taken as evidence of the influence of shared environment (defined as any environmental influence on a trait that causes two or more persons who were reared together to be more alike, on average, than persons who were not reared together.)

Join now!

Lohelin, Horn and Willerman found, in their study on adoptive children, that the level of the birth mother’s IQ had a significant effect on the average IQs of the adopted children, whereas the level of the adoptive families’ socioeconomic status (SES) did not, although there was about a 1-IQ point average difference in favour of the higher SES homes, which might let us think that a favourable environment gives genotypes more freedom to express themselves, whereas a very unfavourable one would probably be restrictive. They also found that the resemblance in IQ levels, even between biological children and their parents, ...

This is a preview of the whole essay