Reactive Attachment Disorder

Authors Avatar

Reactive Attachment Disorder and its Representation in

Eastern European Children in Institutional Care

Reactive attachment disorder (RAD) refers to an unrelenting and severe disturbance in a child's ability to interact and relate to others across social situations (Hanson & Spratt, 2000). The onset of RAD typically occurs before the age of five, and begins in a child's earliest relationships with caregivers. It is to be differentiated from autism and pervasive developmental disorders by its distinctively social origins and the child's relatively normal cognitive functioning (Zeanah & Emde, 1985).  

The prevalence of reactive attachment disorder is unclear. Attachment disorders are commonly misconstrued and under-diagnosed. The symptoms begin early and they often become pervasive throughout life; the disorder may bear a resemblance to many others by the time they are recognized (Sheperis, Renfro-Michel & Doggett, 2003). Although a significant amount of research has been reported on attachment between parents and infants, only a minority of researchers have investigated the disorder that results when the attachment process is disturbed.

Inter-country adoption is a progressively more common manner of family formation, with an annual rate of approximately 2000 adoptions in Canada over the past decade (Citizenship and Immigration Canada, 2003). Research suggests that the majority of these children do very well in their new homes. For example, a Quebec study (Tessier, Larose, Moss, et al, 2005) which surveyed children adopted between 1985 and 2002 found that, although there is a great variability of outcome among inter-country adopted children, as a whole these children show rates of attachment security and social adaptation that is comparable to the general population of the province. Nevertheless, it must be recognized that some children adopted from foreign countries arrive in their new families with special needs.

Using recent research, this paper will provide an overview of reactive attachment disorder, its symptoms, characteristics and causal factors and a look into the susceptibility of institutionalized infants and children. It will conclude with a discussion of the current evaluation and treatment implements.

Theories of Attachment

John Bowlby developed the theory of attachment that underscored the significance of the relationship between a mother and her infant. His theory has since been confirmed and it is now realized that this bond influences a child’s ability to develop quality relationships throughout life (Maroney, 2001). Bowlby's theory of attachment was established upon evolutionary thinking. Infants are vulnerable and incapable of fending for themselves. Thus, the attachment process is designed to ensure the survival of the infant and, in turn, the species (Haugaard & Hazan, 2004). Given that an infant is well loved and its biological needs are reliably met, the child will learn to trust and feel secure with his caregiver, and a healthy attachment will be made (Wilson, 2001). This attachment will persistently influence one's interpersonal relationships throughout life.

In 1978, Ainsworth and her colleagues expanded on Bowlby's work with the idea that the primary caregiver acts as a secure base for exploration. They established the theory of “caregiver sensitivity to infant signals and the role of this sensitivity in the development of attachment patterns”(Wilson, 2001, p.38). Using what is referred to as “the strange situation”, they developed a method for assessing the quality of attachment. This laboratory procedure characterizes the attachment patterns corresponding to the infant’s reactions to reunion with the caregiver. Infants fall into three distinct attachment patterns: secure, insecure-resistant and insecure-avoidant (Wilson, 2001). The amalgamation of Bowlby’s attachment theory and the findings of the strange situation opened the door for research into infants and adolescents who present attachment problems. Securely attached infants demonstrate little avoidance or resistance to contact with the caregiver and draw on the caregiver as a secure base for exploration. Insecure-avoidant infants demonstrate avoidance of contact with the caregiver and often show little preference for the caregiver over a stranger. Insecure-resistant infants demonstrate resistance to contact with the caregiver and frequently show greater hostility and ambivalence than infants in the other two groups (Wilson, 2001).

Reactive Attachment Disorder

In the late 1970s and early 1980s, clinicians and researchers began to recognize that children who encountered abuse, neglect, or recurrent disruptions in primary caregivers often display varying degrees of cognitive, physical and social-emotional problems (Richters & Volkmar, 1994). After numerous modifications, the existing diagnosis of reactive attachment disorder in the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV) characterizes RAD as “markedly disturbed and developmentally inappropriate social relatedness in most contexts” (Zeanah, 2000, p.1). The DSM-IV requires that the behaviour not be solely due to developmental delay or pervasive development disorders. A child diagnosed with RAD must have a history of pathogenic care, meaning experiences of parental/caregiver abuse and neglect or lack of a consistent caregiver (O'Connor T, Bredenkamp D, Rutter M, et al, 1999; Zeanah & Fox, 2004)

The DSM-IV distinguishes between two forms of reactive attachment disorder. The inhibited type is characterized by a failure to suitably initiate and respond to social interactions. These children may have a propensity toward avoidance behaviours (Wilson, 2001). Zeanah, Smyke, & Dumitrescu, (2002) describe children with the inhibited type as lacking the propensity to initiate or respond suitably during social interactions. They may be hypervigilant or highly ambivalent. The disinhibited type is more closely associated with social promiscuity. The child may be excessively social in inappropriate circumstances (Wilson, 2001). A child who is diagnosed with this subtype lacks any preferences when choosing those from whom to seek comfort, support and nurturance. This results in an atypical overfriendliness with relatively unfamiliar adults and has been labelled “indiscriminate sociability” (Zeanah, 2000, p. 1).

Join now!

Symptoms and Characteristics

The symptoms associated with reactive attachment disorder may confuse many mental health counsellors (Lieberman & Zeanah, 1995). Sheperis, Renfro-Michel, et al. (2003) explain that symptoms seem very similar to other childhood disorders and therefore RAD may go undiagnosed. Infants who exhibit a weak crying response, perceptible defensiveness or a failure to thrive may indicate cause for alarm (Solomon & George, 1999). However, many symptoms may not be discernible until the child begins school (Hayes, 1997). These symptoms include low self-esteem, lack of self-control, antisocial attitudes, and aggression. Teachers and other adults may also notice an inability to ...

This is a preview of the whole essay