Report on Psychological Research into Eyewitness Testimony

Authors Avatar

Report on Psychological Research into Eyewitness Testimony

Eyewitness testimony is the study of how accurately a person may recall significant events that they have witnessed taking place.  In the scenario of an individual witnessing a crime it is highly important that the statement they give of information that they remember is accurate, as in a court their evidence will highly influence jurors.  By studying eyewitness testimony psychologists have been able to see the causes of inaccurate reports made by witnesses; this research can show the court that so much weight shouldn’t be placed on an eyewitness’ account, and can help to prevent errors occurring so frequently in the justice system.  The main areas that psychologists believe may affect the memory are reconstructive memory, leading questions in interviews, and effects from fear or anxiety.  Research into these areas allows psychologists to understand how the mind and memories of an eyewitness functions, and so to adjust current methods used within eyewitness testimony accordingly.  This can help to improve the reliability of accounts given by eyewitnesses, using results of relevant investigations as a solid basis.

Reconstructive memory is the theory that memories may be distorted by an individual’s prior knowledge or expectations surrounding an event.  Bartlett (1932) proposed this idea as memory involving active reconstruction and interpretation of events.  Instead of merely recalling the actual facts that we witnessed or know to have happened, memory draws upon understanding and knowledge that we already have in our minds of previous similar events (commonly known as schemas).  The influence of a schema in eyewitness testimony may appear as the witness creates a ‘memory’ of what happened- in the incident a thief may have snatched a woman’s handbag and run away, but due to the previous knowledge and experience stored by a schema that violence is often involved in these types of attacks, the witness may claim to have seen the attacker push the victim, although contact was never made.

A key study into the reconstructive memory and the influence of schemas was performed by Bartlett (1932).  He used a folk story to test the effects of unfamiliarity on the participants’ recall.  Participants, who were all English, were told a traditional North American folk tale known as “The War of the Ghosts”.  The story used words and ideas that the English participants would not be familiar with, as they would not normally feature in conventional Western stories that they may have previously heard.  Bartlett allowed for a 2o hour period to elapse before asking the participants to recall the story; this amount of time would allow any effects of schemas and reconstructive memory to take place.  After the initial recall, participants were asked to repeat the story a number more times.

Bartlett found that participants changed the original story in many ways.  The language and narrative techniques they used were typical of their cultural and literacy background.  More importantly; rationalizations, omissions, changes of passage order, alterations in the importance of sections, and distortions of characters’ emotions were made.  Each time the story was retold, participants would make further adjustments to the text, making it increasingly become a more traditional English story.

This shows how the participants used schemas and reconstructive memory to take the original story and unconsciously rewrite it in their minds to make it more coherent and easily remembered for them.  Ideas that they hadn’t previously experienced were either lost or reformed to fit with their preset expectations.

This experiment is important in explaining the theory of reconstructive memory, and it also shows the unreliability of eyewitness testimony.  If an unfamiliar story can be changed unconsciously to such a degree after being heard only once, it suggests that after experiencing an unfamiliar event, such as an attack or theft, the memory could ultimately do the same.  This would mean that details an eyewitness has provided cannot be entirely trusted due to the nature of the human memory and the concept of reconstructive memory.

Gauld and Stephenson (1967) criticised Bartlett’s study as they found that if the participants were told that accurate recall was important then the numbers of errors made were significantly reduced.  However, in the case of eyewitness testimony the effect may not be the same.  The witness usually understands the importance of accuracy in the information they are giving and is often reminded of this fact, yet real life cases and laboratory studies have shown that recall is still not entirely accurate.

It has also been said that due to the nature of Bartlett’s experiment the results may not be consistent.  He used a story that he considered to be meaningless to people living in England, although this assumption may be correct there was no way of measuring how meaningless it was to each individual.  Due to this people may have been able to relate, or not to relate to the story, across a spectrum; this may have altered the results without explanation- people who understood a certain part of the story better than others were more likely to remember it.

        Further investigation was done into schemas in 1981 by Brewer and Treyens whose aim it was to find the effects of schemas on visual memory.  Their experiment involved showing 30 participants a room one by one, each person stayed in the room for 35 seconds.  The room they entered appeared like an office, containing 61 items- some that one would expect in an office (e.g. a desk, a calendar or a typewriter), some that were more unusual (e.g. a pair of pliers, a skull and a brick).  In recall tests it was found that participants were easily able to remember the standard office items, however were less successful at naming the random items.

This was due to the schema expectancy- the regular items had high schema expectancy for an office and so were remembered accurately.  The positions of items recalled were also adjusted to fit the participant’s schema, such as the notepad being said to be placed on the desk when it was in fact on the seat.

Brewer and Treyens found that the participants automatically used schemas in order to remember the items in the room.  They assumed due to their schemas that the room was an office, and so when asked to recall the items in the room, used knowledge from their schema of standard office items.  This led to the random items being forgotten and other objects that didn’t exist being created.

Once again this shows how eyewitness testimony may be affected.  The schema a witness has of an event may affect the accuracy of their recall when retelling the event.  There is no guarantee of accuracy in their statement, but witnesses are still used as one of the most reliable sources of information in court cases.

        Schemas are not the only influence over a person’s memory when trying to recall an event; they may become entwined with false or misleading information presented by others.  Loftus (1993) demonstrated this in an experiment known as ‘Lost in the Mall’.  The aim of the investigation was to show how the human mind can be manipulated initially by others, and then continually by schemas to form false memories of happenings that never occurred.

Loftus used five participants in the pre-test experiment, selecting university students to be the experimenters.  Over the Thanksgiving holidays they would imprint false memories on a younger sibling (the participant) and then record the results.

One of the children, named Chris, was convinced over the vacation by his older brother, Jim, that he had been lost in a mall at the age of five.  Just two days after being told of this false event for the first time, Chris was able to recall the “memory” in great detail.  He was reported by Jim to have said, “That day I was so scared I would never see my family again.  I knew that I was in trouble.

Join now!

The next day he was also able to recall conversations with his mother from after the mall scenario, claiming, “I remember mom telling me never to do that again.

The memory appeared to be stronger after a few weeks had passed and Chris was invited to the laboratory.  He was able to recall specific pieces of information in great detail, fully believing that the situation had actually occurred, “I was with you guys for a second and I think I went over to look at the toy store, the Kay Bee toy and uh, we got lost, and I was ...

This is a preview of the whole essay