Sleepiness - A critique of its measurement

Authors Avatar

Sleepiness – A critique of its measurement

Contents

Introduction        

Overview of Sleep Function        

The different dimensions of Sleepiness        

Overview: methods for measuring Sleepiness        

SUBJECTIVE MEASURES        

Visual Analogue Scales        

Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS)        

Stanford Sleepiness Scale (SSS)        

Karolinska Sleepiness Scale (KSS)        

OBJECTIVE MEASURES        

EEG        

Pupillography        

Vigilance and Reaction Time        

Maintenance of Wakefulness (MWT)        

Multiple Sleep Latency Test (MSLT)        

The MSLT looks at sleep propensity, i.e. it conceptualises sleepiness as the likelihood of falling asleep. It has proved extremely popular with sleep researchers, referred to by Roehrs, et al. (2000) as the “the standard physiological measure of sleepiness” (see Carskadon et al., 1986). The test uses standard polysomnographic techniques to measure the latency to fall asleep, on repeated measures at 2-hour intervals during the day. The setting is a quiet, dark bedroom.        

Other behavioural components… moving on to Johns’ stuff        

Jonathan Scott        

Prof Jim Horne, Prof Kevin Morgan & Dr Clare Anderson        

Sleep Research Laboratory, Department of Human Sciences, Loughborough University, Leicestershire, U.K.        

References        

 


Introduction

Sleepiness is certainly associated with such sleep disorders as sleep apnea and narcolepsy, but it is also a phenomenon that ‘intrudes’ into the waking state of ‘healthy sleepers’, who may or may not have experienced some loss to their normal daily quota of sleep in the preceding night(s). There is strong evidence that there is a high incidence of road accidents where sleepiness is the most likely cause (Horne & Reyner, 1995; Lyznicki et al., 1998), and, more broadly, that sleepiness has an important impact on general health (Dement & Mitler, 1993; Briones et al., 1996). There is however discordance over what exactly sleepiness is and how to measure it (Johns, 1998). These are issues of utmost importance. Clearly, to be able to successfully manage sleepiness, we need to have a sophisticated understanding of the phenomenon, and to do so, we need effective measurement tools.

This essay will constitute a critique of the measurement of sleepiness. However the author will not take the usual approach of looking at the individual measurement tools (i.e. methods) used by researchers in this endeavour and their relative strengths and weaknesses. Instead, it is proposed that a richer critique might be achieved through viewing the measurement of sleepiness as encompassing both; 1) the methods the researcher chooses to examine this phenomenon; and 2) the researcher’s interpretation of the results. The underlying assumption made by adopting this approach is that measurement can be seen to be multi-dimensional, consisting of the tool(s) used by the researcher and the practice in which the researcher engages when interpreting the results obtained. Theorising as to how these two aspects of research interact should form the basis for some interesting discussion. The multiple sleep latency test (MSLT) will be focused on in a little bit more depth than the other tools as the author believes the MSLT not only provides a good basis for understanding sleepiness and its measurement, but will also allow us to look at the interplay between the two aforementioned dimensions of measurement. This essay will conclude by observing some present research endeavours pertaining to sleepiness and by contemplating what the future of the measurement of sleepiness might be.

Some Guidelines

Rather than attempt to define sleepiness thoroughly here at the start, the author will offer a simple working definition, more through necessity than volition. Until further elaboration ensues, ‘sleepiness’ is proposed to be simply ‘the phenomenon associated with a lack of sleep, as experienced during wakefulness, distinct to tiredness and fatigue’. The distinction between sleepiness and tiredness is an important one; they are taken to be qualitatively, as opposed to quantitatively different (see Horne, 2003). But this is just one aspect of the definition that deserves elaboration. Indeed, a second theme of this essay will be to elaborate on this definition once we have unravelled some of the complexities associated with the phenomenon of sleepiness. Before continuing, it is necessary to state that this essay will explore issues pertaining to human sleepiness. And furthermore, as experienced in young adults.

Overview of Sleep Function

Some of the discordance over what sleepiness is and how it should be measured is rooted in the wider, on-going debate over what exactly the function of sleep is. Approaching sleepiness from this perspective should thus be a useful starting point. However, an extensive discussion on this topic will be avoided, mainly because the parameters of this essay mandate such. Excellent summaries already exist on the different perspectives on sleep function (Rechtschaffen, 1998) and how they relate to the measurement of sleepiness (Horne, 1991). Providing a brief and crude overview of two different schools of thought will suffice.

Physiological Need State

Webb and Agnew (1975) made the controversial claim that maybe we should be aiming to sleep 9-10 hours a night, rather than the accepted average of 7.5hours. As support for their claim, the researchers reported that the young adults who had participated in their study had slept on an extra 126 minutes on average when allowed to ‘oversleep’ in the morning. The researchers tried to draw credence to their hypothesis by commenting that on awakening many people do not feel fully refreshed. Furthermore they draw support to their argument from a report written in 1910, claiming that it documented that the young adult slept an average of nine hours. Consequently, Webb and Agnew proposed that most of us are “chronically sleep deprived”, i.e. the capacity to take more sleep is indicative of a physiological need to do so. This is a consensus shared by various researchers (Carskadon & Dement, 1982). We shall return to this proposition a little later.

A very different outlook is offered by Horne (1988), who compares sleeping to eating and drinking. He suggests that our daily intake of food and water invariably consists of quantities of food and drink that exceed a real physiological requirement. This behaviour is partly accounted for by social facilitation and is more likely to reflect some aspect of humans’ willingness to indulge in an activity for the pure pleasure that it allows. Horne proposes that the same might be said for sleep, i.e. that while a certain amount is undoubtedly necessary physiologically, there is an ability to take more than this (i.e. ‘overeat’ or ‘oversleep’) and this is largely a behavioural phenomenon. Accepting Horne’s analogy to eating and drinking as a useful explanatory concept, it follows that sleepiness is likely to have various dimensions. Let us inspect the evidence that is relevant to our focus on sleepiness and its measurement.

Join now!

The different dimensions of Sleepiness

Horne (1998) argues that there are two modes of sleep, “Core” and “Optional” sleep. Core sleep refers to an essential component, that is; necessary for the maintenance of normal psychological performance; made up largely by delta electroencephalonographic (EEG) activity, or slow wave sleep (SWS), and also about half of our nightly rapid eye movement (REM) sleep; largely concentrated in the first three or four sleep stages (i.e. the initial 4-5 hours of sleep). Optional sleep basically constitutes the remainder of sleep and is appears to be more dispensable. It is extremely important to ...

This is a preview of the whole essay