What are the Risk Factors for Childhood and Adolescent Depression?

Authors Avatar

Jackie Tilston – Extended Essay – 2001

What are the Risk Factors for

Childhood and Adolescent Depression?

PS51010a

Extended Essay

Name:                Jackie Tilston

Date:                30 March 2001


Popular psychology tends to blame childhood and adolescent depression on factors such as experience of divorce.  In reality however the risk factors for depression across these ages are much more complicated.  There are many perspectives that have been used to address childhood and adolescent depression, including developmental, psychobiological, and psychodynamic approaches.  There are therefore a number of different levels of explanation and many specific explanations within each level.  Although not all explanations can be discussed here, it is the aim of this essay to extract as many of the risk factors as possible that overlap at least one of these levels.  To arrive at as full a description as possible it is necessary to ask why it is useful to analyse what causes depression in children and adolescents, both theoretically and practically.  It is then necessary to, firstly, examine what is meant by depression generally and, secondly, to briefly analyse what the differences are between adult depression and depression in young people, before providing a short description of the parameters of childhood and adolescence.  The risk factors or causes of depression within these parameters will then be described, including gender, hormones, genetic contributions, stress, attachment issues and maternal depression.  It should be noted that space does not permit an exhaustive analysis of all risk factors, rather the most salient causes have been extracted.  Finally, an attempt will be made to answer the question of whether there are any implications of risk factors for treatment.  

One theoretical reason why it is useful to analyse what causes depression is because up until recently it was not uncommon for papers on childhood and adolescent depression to come from only one perspective of psychology and for this to have detrimental implications.  For example, the orthodox psychoanalytic perspective was taken and then that theory applied to the likelihood and characteristics of depression in children of different ages (Cicchetti & Schneider-Rosen, 1986).  This particular perspective has led to such statements as made by Rie (1966; in Cicchetti & Schneider-Rosen, 1986): prepubertal children are unable to be depressed because they do not have a fully developed ego.  This view has been expressed in other ways too.  Malmquist’s claims (1971, 1975; in Cicchetti & Schneider-Rosen, 1986, p. 74) have been described thus: “… children’s lack of ego differentiation limits their range of potential affective expressions”.  Anthony (1975; in Cicchetti & Schneider-Rosen, 1986) supported this view by proposing that as children were more easily influenced by their environment their depressive reactions were transient and brief.  In summarising and further supporting these claims to a certain extent, Cicchetti & Schneider-Rosen (1986) propose that any model of childhood depression should take account cognitive development, thus implying that depression cannot occur in children whose cognitions are not sufficiently complex.  

It should be noted that clearly these points of view have important practical implications for treatment – would a young child fitting a depressive diagnosis not be treated appropriately if they were considered to be too young to actually be depressed?  This view, though dominant for some time, has now fallen out of favour (Greenberg et al., 1990 in Graham & Easterbrooks, 2000).  Furthermore, studies have now found that children can indeed suffer depression, for example the so-called Isle of Wight Studies (Rutter, 1979; Rutter et al. 1970; in Rutter, 1986).  This question is just one justification for meticulous study of the etiology of depression in children and adolescence.  More recently it has been found that childhood onset depression has a 60 to 70 % risk of continuing into adulthood and 20 to 40 % of developing into bipolar disorder within five years (Weller & Weller, 2000).  Both these reasons have implications for treatment and as etiology may affect accurate diagnosis and treatment efficacy these reasons provide further support for an integrative study of the risk factors of depression in children and adolescents.

Before looking at what might cause depression in young people it is worthwhile to briefly look at what depression is – its symptoms and special points relating to children and adolescents.  In a general discussion of depression Hammen (1997) describes depression under four categories: affective, cognitive, behavioural and physical symptoms.  These symptoms might include listlessness and apathy; irritability (especially in children); feelings of guilt, thoughts of a bleak future, difficulty in concentrating; behavioural symptoms include an inability to get out of bed, psychomotor changes; and physical symptoms could include changes in appetite, sleep and energy.  Weinberg et al. (1978; in Poznanski, 1979) set the following as criteria for the diagnosis of childhood depression.  He required: 1) dysphoric mood, and 2) self-deprecatory ideation.  Two or more of the following were also required: 1) aggressive behaviour; 2) sleep disturbance; 3) change in school performance; 4) diminished socialisation; 5) change in attitude toward school; 6) somatic complaints; 7) loss of usual energy, and 8) unusual change in appetite or weight.  Additionally, these symptoms needed to remain for at least a month.  In their review of patterns of emotion in depression, Izard & Schwartz (1986) describe studies that have analysed depressive emotions such as anger, sadness and detachment in infants and young children.  Interestingly, emotions of fear, shame and guilt – common in adolescent and adult depression – have not been detected in children (Scarr & Salapatek, 1970; in Izard & Schwartz, 1986) and it has been suggested that this is because children’s cognitive development is not high enough to have developed a stable enough self-image, on which these emotions might be based (Groh, 1980; in Izard & Schwartz, 1986).  It has only been relatively recently (since the 1970s) that it has been widely accepted that depression exists more than rarely in children (Rutter, 1986).  While some researchers have maintained that little difference exists between adult and youth depression, it has been asserted that there are in fact differences in the form and frequency of depression between these two groups (Rutter, 1986).  

Join now!

Emde et al. (1986) chose to define childhood as encompassing four periods: infancy, preschool, school age and adolescence.  These periods coincide with (Emde et al., 1986, p. 141): “(1) traditional divisions with respect to society’s role expectations, (2) theoretical divisions based on major transformation in cognitive development as demarcated by Piaget (Piaget & Inhelder, 1969), and (3) theoretical divisions of psychosexual development as demarcated by Freud (1905/1953) and Erikson (1950).”  Within each of these groups, prevalence has been found to be as follows.  In children younger than 12, the incidence of depression is said to be about 2 to ...

This is a preview of the whole essay