Review of colour constancy in human visual system

Authors Avatar

Introduction

Object surfaces differ in how they can absorb and reflect light [Brainard, 2003]. The spectral distribution of the light that reaches the eye under specific lighting conditions is known as luminance. In the simplest model, luminance is determined by the spectral power distribution (colour) and angle of the light reaching the object, and the reflectance properties of the object in the scene. More complex models also account for the atmosphere the light travels through.[Adelson, 1999] Human beings are able to see the same scene under different illumination conditions, and are still able to maintain a perceptual representation of scenes that remains stable against these changes in illumination [Kraft and Brainard, 1999]. This phenomenon is known as colour constancy. Colour constancy is essential if colour information is to be used as a useful variable in the identification of an object, as if something appears to be a different colour under differential lighting conditions, colour cannot be a useful perceptual indicator of object properties.

Human vision traditionally exhibits good colour constancy across changes in illuminant spectral power distribution [McCann 1976, Kraft and Brainard 1999]. The human colour constancy mechanism must cope with two distinct situations in order to be effective: changes in scene illuminant over time (termed successive colour constancy), and areas of the same scene which are lit by illuminants of differing spectral power distribution (known as simultaneous colour constancy) [Brainard, 2003]. The mechanisms that govern these two phenomena need neither be the same nor mutually exclusive. In order to be colour constant, the visual system must be able to discount the variations in illumination across scenes so as to determine the reflectance of a given surface with a degree of constancy.  The visual system is composed of low, mid and high level processing centres that may all have role to play in this image analysis. This review looks at experiments conducted to test varying hypothesis on the mechanisms behind this area of colour vision, and then relates these to the project work to be undertaken. The project is essentially a study into the ability of the visual system to discriminate illuminant boundaries, tested by measuring the degree of simultaneous colour constancy across two simulated illuminants.

The first section of this review looks at the models proposed for the colour constancy phenomenon. Work into lightness perception is also introduced, and its relevance discussed. Mechanistic and computational models are compared, and their successes and failures are examined. The second section describes the experiment and the aims for the project.

Colour constancy mechanisms

Visual information about a scene is detected by the retina, which soon after a great amount of image compression through various mechanistic models passes the information to the visual system via the optic nerve and the thalamus [Kandell, 2000]. The retina is capable of discriminating differing wavelengths of light by virtue of its three classes of cone photoreceptors [Kandell, 2000]. This raw data does not however provide direct clues about the spectral power distribution of the illuminant. This is explained in figure 1.  

The colour signal (luminance) reflected back to the observer is not useful by itself; to achieve colour constancy the illuminant variances need to be discounted in order to normalise the information about object reflectance.[Helmholtz, 1962] Initially this may seem a paradox, as the raw data cannot be decomposed once confounded by the retina. Indeed it is possible to simulate conditions where two images may appear entirely alike to an observer, but in actuality comprise alternate illuminants and surfaces respectively. [Brainard, 2003]. Models attempting to explain colour and lightness constancy propose systems that can use information across the whole scene to overcome this problem, and make an educated guess as to what the illuminant may have been. This is known as the Illumination Estimation hypothesis, and is generally considered the broad method by which the visual system maintains constancy. However recent work by Rutherford et al (2002) into lightness constancy showed that the Illumination estimation hypothesis failed to perform as expected, potentially calling this pillar of research into doubt. However doubts remain with the experimenters as to the experimental design, and further work needs to be done to address this issue further.

In searching for potential mechanisms to explain the phenomenon, colour constancy has been approached from two key directions, that of a mechanistic approach, and that of a computational one. Mechanistic theories attempt to explain constancy phenomena by ascribing data analysis and processing to simple visual mechanisms at the cellular level. One such theory was proposed by Von Kries (Brainard, 2003), and is called the Von Kries adaptation model. This model takes information from the initial encoding of the colour signal at the cones, and ascribes each type of cone (L, M and S) a quantal absorption rate for a given luminance. The photons that make up a luminance are therefore grouped into their relative stimulation of 3 classes of cone. The model demonstrates that when the signals are adapted through a vector transformation, quantal absorption rates can be held relatively constant across illumination changes by controlling changes in signal gain. Standardisation can be potentially achieved by applying a linear model [Wandell, 1995] to the luminance data that takes into consideration the standard spectral composition of daylight. Judd et al [1964] measured the spectral composition of daylight around the world by recording reflected light off a surface of known achromatic reflectance. They discovered that daylight is relatively constant. This finding provides a reference databank for the most likely illuminant to be encountered by the visual system.

Join now!

 This is evidence for a mechanistic system behind successive colour constancy. This gain control model gives a good approximation of constancy across illumination variance across scenes, but lacks an explanation of the factors that control this adaptation. Gilchrist et al, 1999 has applied this model to the lightness constancy phenomena closely associated with colour constancy (where the perceived reflectance of a surface remains constant over luminance changes). He found that such a model would need to find a reference lightness, and “anchor” all other measurements to this reference. This is referred to as an anchoring rule. How this happens ...

This is a preview of the whole essay