It is clear that physical activity can be used in conjunction with other methods to help improve mood and there are many benefits to this ‘treatment’. Physical activity is cheap compared to other forms of treatment, there are no real side effects if it is administered correctly and most importantly it can be self sustained and maintained across a person’s full life cycle, which is most certainly beneficial to the person’s future health. However, there is no specific guidelines to say that to improve mood you are required to run, jog, skip or swim a certain distance for a certain duration of time, at a specific intensity. Many studies have been completed which provide a clearer understanding of which types of activities are most beneficial to enhancing mood and the best intensity and duration these activities should be completed at.
Osei-tutu and Campagna (2004) completed a study comparing the effect of long and short bout exercise on mood. The study comprised of three different study groups, long bout (LB), short bout (SB) and a sedentary control group (CTL). All the subjects used in the study were defined as sedentary, as they had not completed regularly in physical activity, 3 x 20 minutes per week, in over six months. Both LB and SB study groups completed a training regime which consisted of 30 min of walking on 5 days of the week for 8 weeks. Subjects were requested to keep their heart rate at a constant level, 60-79% of their maximum heart rate. The LB study group completed the 30 minutes of exercise in one single bout at a time of their choice, opposed to the SB study group who were required to complete duration of the exercise in three 10 minute bouts which were separated with an interval of ≥ 2 hours. Mood was measured using POMS and pre-, mid- and post mood was assessed after the exercise sessions on weeks 4 and 8.
The results gained from the study completed by Osei-tutu and Campagna (2004) are shown in appendix 1. These show that overall physical activity does help improve mood states. Osei-tutu and Campagna (2004) claim that, on the whole, LB and SB exercise brings a significant decrease (P ≤ 0.01) in total mood disturbance. Appendix 1 also shows that LB exercise is slightly more beneficial than SB exercise, however it can also be viewed that leading a sedentary life style does not improve mood states. With comparison to the base line scores which were gathered at the start of the study, post mood scores in the LB exercise group show the most significant change, (P ≤ 0.05.) In spite of this, these results alone do not simply prove that LB exercise is more beneficial because the chart takes into account all of the 6 mood states measured and totals the scores together. Appendix 2 provides more accurate information as it simply analyses the effect exercise has on vigour, the only positive mood state taken into consideration in POMS. Appendix 2 shows that LB exercise significantly improves vigour and that in turn, with consideration to the iceberg profile; means that ones mood state has improved and became more positive. It is evident to see that completing an activity continuously for 30 minutes does improve vigour a great deal more than completing physical activity for 30 minutes in three 10 minute periods over a day. It can be argued however that although it is not as beneficial as long bouts of exercise, it is still very effective and can be used to help improve mood.
The study completed by Osei-tutu and Campagna (2004) aids in explaining how it is duration that is most effective to improve mood state. Osei-tutu and Campagna (2004) identify that there are a number of variables which can affect an individuals mood state including listening to music and having a conversation with someone else, so by removing these variables more accurate results are provided. A problem with the study, on the other hand, is that there was no supervision of the subjects whilst they were completing the physical activity, therefore there is no evidence to say that the subjects were not listening to music whilst walking or walking with a partner or friend. As well as this there is no evidence to say that the subjects actually took part in the walking exercise for the correct length of time. The subjects used in the study were sedentary subjects so at the time of the study they were in the stages of precontemplation, with no intention to become active. It therefore would have been a very difficult task for the subjects to become active and stick to the program.
A study completed by McGowan and Talton (1996) provided evidence to support the study conducted by Osei-tutu and Campagna (2004) that one single bout of physical activity is more beneficial than more than one bout of physical activity per day. The study by McGowan and Talton (1996) however does not have any comparison with multiple physical activity bouts. The only two groups used in their study were the single bout of exercise group and a control group who sat in on a lecture about sports psychology. McGowan and Talton (1996, p863) do, however state that the after an acute bout of physical activity participants tended to feel better.
The intensity of physical activity is also considered another variable which is important with relation to mood states. Dishman (1986) is cited in Berger and Owen (1998, p612) saying that high intensity (≥ 80% VO² max) is associated with improved mood states, however this is contradicted my many. Steptoe and Cox (1988) cited in Berger and Owen (1998, pp; 612) state that “high intensity exercise was associated with short term increases in ratings of tension and decreases in vigour”, which overall means that mood was not improving due to high intensity exercise.
Berger and Owen (1998) conducted a study comparing the difference between, low, moderate and high intensity exercise to provide an indication of which intensity level is most beneficial to improving mood states. It was highlighted before the study by Berger and Owen (1998) that activities completed at a moderate intensity were beneficial to increasing mood state, however there was no evidence to say that it was significantly more beneficial or at all more beneficial than low or high intensity levels.
The study completed by Berger and Owen (1998) consisted of 91 subjects which were split up into 4 study groups. The study groups used in this study were low, moderate and high intensity groups and a control group was also studied. Subjects were requird to complete a 14 week program which consisted of a body conditioning class twice a week, taking place at the same time every week. The subjects in the low, moderate and high intensity groups were asked to complete the exercise class at 50%, 65% and 80%, of their maximum heart rate (HR max), respectively. It was reported, however, that problems occurred with the heart rate intensity levels and the high intensity group were working at a rate of 79% of HR max, which according to Corbin and Pangrazi (1996) cited in Berger and Owen (1998) is classed as a moderate intensity level. This resulted in the study becoming a comparison of low intensity and two categories of moderate intensity levels. Mood was measured using POMS before and after the exercise class on weeks 6, 9 and 12.
The results gathered from the study provided strong evidence that low intensity levels were more beneficial than either of the moderate intensity levels. The results showed that low intensity significantly improved the 5 negative mood states when comparing pre mood state and post mood state, however moderate intensity at 65% HR max did prove to provide the best results in improving vigour. Nevertheless, the results from the study showed that no matter what level of intensity exercise was completed at; there were overall improvements of mood state.
The study completed by Berger and Owen (1998) can be positively viewed as there were a number of variables kept constant when the study was conducted. Variables such as the same instructor used for each class and the time of day the class were assigned for meant that mood could be precisely assessed. On the other hand, there is no indication of what level of experience the subjects had previously had in physical activity. If the group was well mixed with experienced and inexperienced subjects then the results may not be as accurate as first thought. Cox (2002, p180) states that those people who take part in physical activity and sport more regularly have a more positive mood state. They exhibit lower negative mood states and a higher vigour scores. With this in mind it could mean that the reason that moderate intensity scores did not increase as much at low intensity was because there were more subjects in the group that had more physical activity experience resulting smaller increases of mood scores as a result of the exercise class.
As explained, it is evident that physical activity does contribute in improving mood state. There are many mechanisms which have been linked to the role which physical activity has on reducing mood state. According to Biddle (2000) there are a number of biological and physiological reasons are behind the role physical activity has on improving mood state, including the thermogenic hypothesis and the endorphin hypothesis.
The thermogenic hypothesis is the theory which suggests that as a result of physical exercise, the bodies’ core temperature will increase and this will result in mood becoming more positive. Craft and Perna (2004) explain that increases of specific areas in the brain such as the brain stem can reduce negative feelings which affect mood such as depression and tension and make the person more relaxed, which in turn will improve their mood state.
The endorphin hypothesis is a theory which according to Biddle (2000) suggests that there is an increased amount of β-endorphin, a neuropeptide produced by the vertebrate brain, due to exercise. Craft and Perna (2004) elaborate by explaining that endorphins are related to a positive mood and an increase in β-endorphin can lead to a more positive mood state, and general increase in well being. It can be argued, however, that these mechanisms presented are relatively new proposals and therefore information and research on the relationship between biological and physiological changes in the body is limited and somewhat unknown. This argument is supported by Craft and Perna (2004) who propose that the serotonin hypothesis is the increase in neurotransmitters which help diminish negative mood state, however, it is later explained that evidence of this has only been found in some animals.
In conclusion, it is appropriate to comment that there is a strong relationship between physical activity and the improvement of mood state (Osei-Tutu and Campagna, 2004; Biddle, 2000; Berger and Owen, 1998). Mental health is a prominent issue in modern day western society, so the development of research in-relation to mental health and physical activity is very productive and proves to be cost friendly to the nation. Studies by Osei-Tutu and Campagna (2004), Berger and Owen (1998) and McGowan and Talton (1996), show that physical activity does help with the improvement of mood. As a result of these studies it is evident that longer bouts of exercise are more beneficial, coupled with low to moderate intensity levels of exercise.
Unfortunately, there is still very little clarification on the recommended activities and sports which will help with mood reduction. Kennedy and Newton (1997, p200) have attempted to address this issue by stating “ it appears that a large variety of activities such as running, swimming, aerobic dancing, cycling and walking are suitable for producing psychological benefits.” Research is limited however, and therefore it would be useful to complete studies which look at the effect team orientated activities such as football and basketball have on mood, but similarly activates such as house work, which is also considered as a form of physical activity.
Appendix
Appendix 1. Bar chart showing total mood distribution. Osei-tutu and Campagna (2004, p96)
Appendix 2. Bar chart showing the change in vigour. Osei-tutu and Campagna (2004, p95)
References
Berger, B.G and Owen, D.R (1998) ‘Relation of low and moderate intensity exercise with acute mood change in college joggers’ in Perfect MOPT skills
Biddle, S.J.H (2000) ‘Emotion, mood and physical activity’ in Biddle, S.J.H, Fox, K.R and Boutcher, S.H Physical Activity and psychological well being. Pp 63-87. London:Routledge
Chambers Dictionary 1998. Chambers:London
Cox, R.H (2002) ‘Mood state and athletic performance’ in Sports Psychology: Concepts and Application. Pp177-190. McGraw-Hill: New York
Craft, L.L and Perna, F.M (2004) ‘The benefits of exercise for the clinically depressed’-
Di Lorenzo, T.M, Bargman, E.P, Stuck-Ropp, R, Brassington, G.S, Frensch, P.A and LaFontaine, T (1999) Long term effects of aerobic exercise on psychological outcomes
Dishman, R.K (1988) ‘Exercise adherence: It’s importance on public health campaign. IL: Human Kinetics
Hale, B.S and Raglin, J.S (2002) ‘State anxiety responses to acute resistance training and step aerobic exercise across 8-weeks of training’ in Journal of sports medicine & physical fitness. Volume 42, part 1. pp 108-112
Kennedy, M.M and Newton, M (1997) ‘Effect of exercise intensity on mood in step aerobics’ in Journal of sports medicine and physical fitness. Volume 37, part 3. pp 200-204
McGowan, R.W and Talton, B.J (1996) ‘Changes in scores on the profile of mood states following a single bout of physical activity: Heart rate and changes in affect’ in Perceptual and motor skills. Volume 38. pp 859-866
Osei-tutu, K.B and Campagna, P.D ‘The effects of short- vs. long-bout exercise on mood, VO2 max, and percent body fat’
Steptoe, A and Cox, S (1988) ‘Accute effects of aerobic exercise on mood’ in Health psychology. Pp 329-340