It is incorrect to draw a sharp distinction between Eastern and Western cultures and ways of thinking - Discuss.

Authors Avatar
"It is incorrect to draw a sharp distinction between Eastern and Western cultures and ways of thinking." Discuss.

Our cultures permeate our everyday lives; influencing the way we behave, make decisions and of course the ways in which we think and process information. However, is there such an explicit distinction between the thought patterns of a "Western" individual and those of someone from an "Eastern" background?

Firstly, it is important to define what we mean by "Western" and "Eastern", although this is a difficult task as there is no clear-cut definition of either. Generally speaking, the term "Western" encompasses those from Europe, the United States, Canada and Australasia, and "Eastern" includes peoples of Asia, the Far East and the Middle East. However, the lack of an unambiguous definition produces an even greater quandary as to whether we can draw a sharp distinction between Eastern and Western cultures and ways of thinking. It is equally important to identify what we mean by "culture", which we are able to define more easily as "the enduring behaviours, ideas, attitudes and traditions shared by a large group of people and transmitted from one generation to the next" (Myers, 2008). Knowing what each of these terms means, we can now attempt to discover whether Eastern and Western cultures and ways of thinking really do differ so greatly.

Let us initially consider the view that it would be incorrect to divide Eastern and Western cultures so distinctly. Many cross-cultural social psychologists have found that an "essential universality" (Lonner, 1980) exists between cultures despite apparent differences. For example, there are five main personality traits that are universally used to describe others throughout the world: stable, agreeable, outgoing, open and conscientious (John & Srivastava, 1999; McCrae & Costa, 1999). Despite the differences in results (Australians being generally more outgoing than others and Canadians being found to be more agreeable), the fact that the five traits are universal means that we cannot immediately draw a sharp distinction between East and West. In addition, it is hard to make such generalized sweeping statements that apply to either the whole of the West or the whole of the East. In fact, our ways of thinking are individual, and individuals differ, making our preferences personal ones rather than cultural ones. For example, some people prefer more personal space1 than others and some people are more punctual than others. These can be seen as individualistic traits rather than traits that originate from either the East or the West.

We may also draw upon socio-biological evidence to prove how similar human beings are. Even humans from different races share 99.9% of their DNA (Astuti, 2008), and therefore, the DNA in a person from the West is only 0.01% different than that of a person from the East. Not only are all humans highly similar genetically, but many psychologists have also shown that everyone has the same basic cognitive processes. Nisbett, in "The Geography of Thought" (2003), writes:
Join now!


"When people in one culture differ from those in another in their beliefs, it can't be because they have different cognitive processes, but because they are exposed to different aspects of the world, or because they have been taught different things" (pp. xiv).

The point that Nisbett makes here is that internally, our bodies are virtually all the same, genetically, due to nature. We only differ because of external or environmental influences - factors of nurture. The 'races' and divisions we create between people of the East and the West are anthropologically referred to as "superficial" (Astuti, ...

This is a preview of the whole essay