Domestic violence. The following essay will concentrate on patriarchal-terrorism (Gilchrist et al. 2004) meaning the non-sexual violence of men against their female intimate partners. This kind of aggression was given many names since its social

Authors Avatar

Introduction

The following essay will concentrate on “patriarchal-terrorism” (Gilchrist et al. 2004) meaning the non-sexual violence of men against their female intimate partners. This kind of aggression was given many names since its “social construction” in the 1970ies (Mehrotra 1999), such as wife-battering, partner or spouse-abuse, family or intimate violence (Straus, 1978, Gondolf, 1988, Gilchrist et al. 2004). Nevertheless, the still most common term “Domestic Violence” (Stanko, 2001, 2004, Hague et al. 2003, Shipway 2004) is used in the following when selected theories of violence in general and Domestic Violence (DV) in particular are presented before a taxonomy of offender-types is introduced and critically evaluated with regard to an appropriate intervention.

Both the extent of Domestic Violence and the risk of serious injuries are high in the UK (Stanko, 2001, Walby & Allen, 2004). One in four women reported physical assault by a current or former partner in their life (Mirrlees-Black, 1999). Nearly, a sixth of all violent crime is domestic (Dodd et al. 2004). In 2001, 2 women were killed every week by their husbands (Stanko, 2001). Around 12, 9 million incidents of Domestic Violence against women occurred in 2003/4 (Walby & Allen. 2004).

As violence-related terms are highly controversial (Berkowitz, 1993), they must be clarified. According to Blackburn (1993: 210), “Violence denotes the forceful infliction of physical injury,” but is only partly committed by aggression which shall be defined as the intentional infliction of harm towards an unwilling object (Geen, 2001, Berkowitz, 1993). Aggression can be divided - although mixed motivations may occur (Anderson & Bushman, 2002) - into instrumental and proactive on the one hand and affective or “hostile” and  reactive on the other (Berkowitz 1993, Archer & Browne, 1989, Crick & Dodge, 1996). The latter may correlate with anger (Geen, 2001) being viewed by Novaco (1978) as an affective stress reaction to provocations. This reaction is composed by a combination of arousal and cognitive labelling as anger.

Recently, the “traditional” assumption that anger causes aggression (Anderson & Bushmann 2002) has been questioned. Berkowitz (2003) assumes that anger plays several causal roles in aggression, such as reducing inhibitions, maintaining aggressive intention over time, priming aggressive scripts and energising behaviour by increasing arousal levels.

Contrarily, hostility is a more permanent negative attitude of resentment, mistrust or even hate (Buss, 1961, Blackburn 1998). On the other hand, aggressiveness refers to a disposition toward becoming aggressive (Blackburn, 1998). However, an act of aggression – defined by Blackburn (1998) as an Individual’s intentional, goal-directed performance– does not have to be connected with a violent disposition. Hence, dispositional and situational factors as well as acquisition, instigation and maintenance of aggression should be distinguished. 

Psychological explanations of general aggressive behaviour

According to some Psychobiologists (Rada, Laws & Rellner 1976, Archer 1988, Moyer, 1981), testosterone-levels are connected with aggressiveness by sensitising the aggression-controlling neural circuits whilst others hold a frontal-lobe dysfunction responsible (Miller et al. 1997, Lau & Phil, 1996).

Sociobiologist Wilson (1975) stresses the evolutionary element in assuming that aggression is applied to support the reproduction of the own genes or those of close relatives – a dubious hypothesis, as most homicides occur intrafamilially (Wolfgang, 1958).

Lorenz‘s (1996) instinct theory views aggression as a result of progressive accumulation of specific energy. As a genetic necessity for vertebrates‘ ritualised defence of territory, it is

discharged when triggered by specific internal stimuli

Kutash (1978) also relies on a psycho-dynamic aggressive drive, but pretends that only those suffering from ego-weakness and lack of self-control are predisposed to an uncontrolled break-out of impulsive violence.

Dollard et al.’s (1939) Frustration-Aggression-Hypothesis relates aggression with previous frustration yielded by external barriers thwarting the goal-directed activity of the later perpetrator. If the source of frustration cannot be attacked because of feared negative consequences, alternative targets are chosen, according the principle of displaced aggression or scape-goating (Hovland and Sears 1940). Berkowitz (1989, 1990, 1993) reformulates this hypothesis in stressing the aversively-stimulated cognitive process. His Cognitive Neoassociation Theory proposes that aversive events such as frustration or provocation produce negative affects, which stimulates cognitive, emotional and physiological responses. High-order cognitive processes include anger-producing attributions, fear or both, which suppress or enhance fight or flight-tendencies. Frustration as one type of provocation increases the level of arousal and leads to the situational trigger of aggression.

In contrast to those approaches neglecting sociocultural, instrumental and dispositional concerns, Bandura’s (1976) „complete“ approach refers to social learning as the origin of aggressiveness. The determination of this is either deliberately or inadvertently received through family or sub cultural peer-influences, the mass media or direct experience. Instigators are modelling influences, aversive treatment and incentive inducements, related with anticipated positive consequences and instructional or bizarre symbolic control. Researches confirmed that individuals behave more punitively after observing aggression (Bandura, 1973).

A higher anger rate enhances the eliciting power of modelling influences as well as cultural justifications or victim’s “invitation” to attack (Berkowitz, 1970). Furthermore, aversive treatment creates a general state of emotional arousal which discharges a broad range of behaviour selected according to the relative effectiveness of learned responses (Bandura, 1973). Aggressive responding is maintained by reinforcement-control such as social status reward  and self-reinforcement. The latter refers to self-reward, self-punishment, its neutralisation by moral justification, euphemistic labelling, displacement of responsibility and victim-blaming (Bandura, 1976). Hence, social learning theory includes both instrumental and hostile aggression by presenting individual‘s acquisition of aggressiveness and his construction of the social world.

On the other hand, Novaco (1978) and other cognitive theorists (Levey and Howells, 1990) focus on the cognitive perception of anger influenced by internal and external factors and their behavioural responses. Hence, according to attribution theorists, anger derives from specific causal attributions (Ferguson and Rule 1983, Weiner, 1986) and is influenced by the judgement of aversion, which depends on individual values, self- esteem and moral development (Kohlberg, 1981). Situational analysis has outlined the instigation of angry violence by verbal or physical provocation (Monahan & Klassen 1982). Thus, the aggressor’s situational attribution is crucial (Luckenbill, 1977). Studies such as Gouze’s (1997), Dodge & Tomlin’s (1987) or Lochman et al.’s (1989) prove the reciprocal, cyclical link between aggressiveness and social competence established by attributing hostile motives to others (Crick & Dodge 1994).

Appropriately, Dodge (1980) brings up hostile attributional biases resulting from defective cognitive processes misinterpreting others’ behaviour and selecting one’s own behaviour from the learned repertoire. Similarly, Huesmann (1998) relates individuals’ reaction to their available scripts defining guide behaviour according to memory-concepts. After selecting a script, the chosen role is performed. So, consisting normative beliefs are established by being retrieved and become chronically accessible (Guerra et al. 1994).

Hence, aggression inhibitions may be overridden, reinforced by social justification of violence and victim blaming (Bandura, 2001). Whilst some populations openly accept certain forms of aggression as legitimate or at least acceptable (Nisbett & Cohen 1996), some subcultures use them to gain excitement, inner-group-status or to express refusal of social conditions (Wolfgang & Ferracutti, 1967). Both are often related with masculinity (Messerschmidt, 1993).

Similarly, interactive approaches concentrate on social relationships of violent offenders towards their environment. Aggression is treated by Felson (1978) as an instrument for impression management. and by Tedeschi & Felson’s (1994) social interaction theory as socially influenced in order to produce a change in the behaviour of the target. Even hostile aggression may contain such a goal, especially if related to perceived threats of unwarranted high- self-esteem or narcissism (Baumeister et al. 1996).

General theories include several approaches. As an example, Anderson & Bushman (2002) focus on knowledge structures containing perceptual and personal schemata next to behavioural scripts. Whilst the latter influence perceptions at multiple levels, the propensity to crime depends on personality traits and attitudes, developed by social learning or genetic predisposition. Accordingly, longitudinal studies (Olweus 1978; 1984, Weiner, 1989) have outlined the developmental consistency of dispositional aggressive patterns. Such findings have justified a concentration on personality traits (Buss & Durkee, 1957; Eyseneck, 1977; Spielberger et al. 1983). Hence, Megargee (1966) distinguishes undercontrolled offenders who are disposed toward weak inhibitions and responding regularly aggressively from overcontrolled ones whose strong inhibitions result in violence only in cases of intensive anger arousal. Contrarily, Barrat (1994) views a combination of high impulsivity and anger as the most powerful antecedents of aggression whilst Caprara et al.’s (1994) study unveiled that irritability, emotional susceptibility and dissipation-rumination predict aggressiveness. Additionally, personality disorders such as Psychpopathy or Antisocial Personality Disorder (APD) have been detected frequently among violent offenders (Powis, 2002).                 

 

However, according to Farrington (1991a, 2001), persistent (violent) perpetrators act predominantly in a versatile way as committing crimes is only one symptom of life-course-persistent antisocial behaviour rooted in childhood and increasing into adulthood. Relevant risk-factors are said to be hyperactivity-impulsiveness-attention deficit, low intelligence, convicted family members, poor parental supervision including harsh or erratic discipline, parental conflict, low family income, poor housing, large family-size, delinquent friends, high delinquency rate at school as well as high crime neighbourhoods (Farrington 2001). Especially negative intrafamilial childhood experiences have been connected with later aggressiveness (Powis, 2002). Longitudinal studies show also a strong continuity from childhood and adolescent antisocial behaviour to later partner abuse across the individual life course which provides conceptual evidence that the causes for Domestic Violence may be the same as for general crime (Capaldi & Clark, 1998; Farrington, 1994; Giardano et al. 1999; Magdol et al. 1998).

A selection of Domestic Violence theories

Despite the supposed coherence between General and Domestic Violence, many specific explanations of the latter exist according to the longstanding conviction that intimate aggression is a unique phenomenon (Moffitt et al. 2000).

Daly and Wilson’s (1989) evolutionary approach makes the male’s sexual proprietary claim responsible. Others (Tornhill & Tornhill 1992) stress the societal attitude, which is attached to cultural norms not only tolerating, but also condoning or extolling aggression (Straus, 1978). Accordingly, feminist approaches view intimate violence as a tool for ensuring men’s domination over women influenced by patriarchal cultural values (Dobash & Dobash 1992; Brownmiller, 1975; Yllö 1993). On the other hand, the prevalence of intimate violence between homosexual couples (Cruz & Firestone, 1998; Girshick, 2002) and the fact that many men do not offend proposes the influence of individual and micro-societal factors (Gilchrist et al. 2004). However, a cultural and situational consideration of men’s inability to accept (feared) loss of authority is required (Dobash & Dobash, 1979, 1992; Birns et al. 1994). Additionally, messages about masculinity animate toughness and emotional distance, which leads to a lack of communication (Cohen, 1990). Albeit violent men seem to be predisposed toward a high commitment to their relationships, they underestimate their partner’s care (Langhinrichsen-Rohling et al. 1994), which supports situational attributions including victim-blaming (Burt 1983; White & Kowalski 1998). Gil (1978) evaluates Domestic Violence as mainly rooted outside the family, but discharged within it because of the informality and the lower risk for punitive sanctions. Goode’s (1971) exchange theory whereupon intrafamilial violence is used to maintain family’s inner order supports this rational-choice perspective (Stephenson, 1992). Gelles & Cornell (1990)’s resource exchange theory makes unpleasant factors such as low wages, poor housing, isolation and alienation at work responsible for frustration or structural stress. Those are violently discharged at home, where, especially if the partner is economically or emotionally dependent (Russell, 1990), the risk of (in-)formal punishment is low, but (immaterial) benefits such as securing the power-position are high.

Join now!

Domestic violence has often been evaluated as an instrument to remedy men’s low self-esteem as a result of a low-status position (Straus et al. 1988; Walker, 1979; Johnston, 1988). On the other hand, Straus et al.’s (1988) observation that the level of violence rises according to the level of woman’s subjugation indicates that high self-esteem leads to violent patronising (Baumeister et al. 1996). Accordingly, Salmivalli (2001) argues that only inflated, narcissistic self-esteem is linked with violent offenders. Moreover, a study of Bushman & Baumeister (1998) distinguishing self-esteem, a cognition, and narcissism, an emotion of self-love, revealed that only narcissism interacted ...

This is a preview of the whole essay