Is language used to label thoughts or are thoughts molded by the formation of language?

Authors Avatar

Benjamin Meza

Period 7

11/05/01

Topic #2: To what extent do the names and labels we use in the pursuit of knowledge affect the conclusions we reach?

Is language used to label thoughts or are thoughts molded by the formation of language?

In the pursuit of knowledge we formulate our ideas by attaching names and labels to the thoughts we conceive.  This dependence on language is common to both thought and communication.  As a form of communication, language is restricted to those universal terms, which can be understood by all people in their limited scope of perception.  Therefore, a precipice is reached where the limitations of language affect formulated concepts, confining them to the minute window of understanding which all humans share.  George Steiner explained it best when he wrote: “Language can only deal meaningfully with a special, restricted segment of reality. The rest, and it is presumably the much larger part, is silence.”  In defining concepts, language also ties meaning to other preconceived definitions and from this there evolves a impedance to true accuracy.  Therefore, it is possible to say that despite the attempt by all languages to describe all possible realities, in the rendering of thoughts the use of names and labels skews to a great extent the realm of possible meaning, often times diverging from one’s original intent.

 In order to develop the way in which names and labels skew our conclusions, one must first analyze their roles in the formation of thoughts.  One major use of words is to attempt o provide an objective description of our environment.  We label something as a “red car” or a “smooth surface,” with the intent of describing an objective environment.  The second is in simplifying concepts as to make them easier to communicate.  This task subdivides names and labels into Generality and Specificity.  By generalizing, one can describe a “wooden shaft encasing a filament of graphite which gradually rubs off when pushed against a textured paper…” as a “pencil.”  The simplification of this concept can be made with a single word.  In specifying, one can break the object down into its components when convenient: “the rubbery material has a stronger attraction to the graphite dust than does the textured material on which you are making the marks and thus removes the dust by a gradual degradation of rubbery shavings.”  A final use of names and labels, which will ultimately influence our conclusions, is in providing relationships to preconceived concepts.  In saying, “a pen is pencil-like but uses ink instead of graphite” the user is defining the new concept of the pen in terms of the already understood concept of the pencil.  These three formations of names and labels are the principal sources of language limitation.  

Join now!

When understanding the use of language in forming knowledge claims, one must find a thought-language relationship.  A general question can define such a relationship: Does language label thoughts and/or are thoughts molded by language?  In solving this problem we know that the former statement is true because a purpose of language is to label our thoughts.  In addition, a good example of the latter being true also is in the history of chemistry. Around the fourth and fifth centuries BC, a great controversy arose over the primordial element, which composed all other forms of matter.  At this point in time, ...

This is a preview of the whole essay