ANIMAL COGNITION CRITICAL REVIEW

Authors Avatar

A critical review on:

Rescorla, R. A. (1988). Pavlovian Conditioning. It’s Not What You Think It Is. American Psychologist, 43(3), 151-160.

In current textbooks that describe how Pavlovian Conditioning works, authors frequently wrote that there are two stimuli in operation – the conditioned stimulus (CS), which is neutral (i.e. elicits no response) and the biologically significant, unconditioned stimulus (US), which is valuable (i.e. elicits an unconditioned response UR). After repeatedly pairing CS and US, previous neutral CS would elicit a conditioned response (CR) which would resemble the UR (Klatsky, 1980; Atkinson, Atkinson, Smith & Hilgard, 1987). Rescorla (1988) argued that wrong information had been written in the textbooks and they have also ignored the dramatic conceptual changes that took place within Pavlovian Conditioning. Rescorla (1988) has also mentioned in his article what the important circumstances for producing Pavlovian Conditioning are, what is learned during conditioning and how conditioning may influence our behaviour. Therefore, this critical review would summarize some of the main claims he has made in the article and provide evidence for and against his claims.

        Firstly, Rescorla (1988) proposed that in order to produce appropriate circumstances for Pavlovian Conditioning to occur, the contiguity of two events (occurring together), in this case the conditioned stimulus (CS) and the unconditioned, biologically significant stimulus (US), is neither necessary nor sufficient for conditioning to occur and for forming an association between CS and US. The failure to arrange contiguity would also not preclude associative learning (Rescorla, 1988). Rather, it should be the information one stimulus (the CS) gives about another (the US), known as the learning of relations or contingency of events, that should be important. The contingency of events (CS and US) can be calculated by the probability of US occurring in the presence of CS minus the probability of US occurring in the absence of CS. It can also be expressed as P(US|CS) – P(US|~CS).

Rescorla (1988) quoted experiments showing that amount of conditioning may be sensitive to the base rate occurrence of US against a CS/US contiguity taking place – P(US|~CS), in which he concluded that contiguity is not a sufficient circumstance for producing conditioning. From Rescorla (1968), asymptotic levels of fear conditioning, measured by the ability of the CS to interfere with the organisms’ ongoing behaviour, can be plotted against P(US|CS) and P(US|~CS). Results revealed that conditioning increased with P(US|CS). However, conditioning was an inverse function of P(US|~CS) while keeping CS/US contiguity constant. By simply increasing the base rate of the US shock, conditioning attenuated from excellent to negligible. Therefore, Rescorla (1988) concluded that conditioning depends not on contiguity between CS and the US, but rather the information CS provides about the US as this captures the relation needed to produce an association between two events.

Rescorla’s (1988) claim was not unchallenged. Some modern theories of associative learning other than Rescorla’s explained effects of CS-US contingency in terms of temporal contiguity (Wasserman, 1989; Gibbon and Balsam, 1981; Jenkins, Barnes and Barrera, 1981). They suggested that a positive CS-US contingency can be said to arrange CSs and USs in a way that the CS signals a shorter time to the US than the average time between USs or the average time between other intermittent stimuli and the US. On the another hand, a negative CS-US contingency can be said to arrange CSs and USs where it takes longer for the CS to signal the US compared with the average time between USs or the average time between other intermittent stimuli and the US. Finally, a zero contingency means to arrange CS s and USs in such a way that the CS signals the same time to the US as the average time between USs or the average time between other intermittent stimuli and the US. Therefore, from the above descriptions, contingency can be expressed in terms of different temporal contiguities which would present a challenge to Rescorla’s (1988) claim that contingency instead of contiguity is the necessary and sufficient circumstance for producing learning, and contingency is perhaps a redundant term this case where contiguity seems sufficient.

Join now!

Moreover, the idea of contingency being a necessary and sufficient circumstance for conditioning by Rescorla (1988) was directly contradicted by results of subsequent experiments (Papini and Bitterman, 1990). In one experiment by Brandon (1981), a single group of pigeons was trained with three colours following each other in a haphazard (random) order. In the presence of colour A, free food was on average given twice per minute. In colour B, once per minute; finally, in colour C, no food was given. Though in all conditions, the probability of reinforcement was exactly the same in its presence as in its ...

This is a preview of the whole essay