Mental imagery can be categorised into 2 terms of perspective. White and Hardy (1995) and Hardy and Callow (1996) describe them like this:
- Internal – imagery is ‘seen’ through the eyes of the athlete – in a first-person view. This is particularly useful for visual-field change activities.
- External – an outside view, like watching yourself from the side-lines. This is useful for technical skill development.
Another major difference between the two types of imagery – internal and external – is that internal imagery allows for kinaesthetic imaging while external imagery does not.
Mahoney and Avener (1977) investigated the use of imagery in elite gymnasts trying out for the Olympic team. They found that the gymnasts who used internal imagery performed the best and got into the Olympic team significantly more than those individuals who did not perform to the required standard to make the Olympic squad. Rotella et al (1980) found the same was also true for high-level skiers. This could mean that internal imagery is the best form of mental imagery but some studies disagree, it could be said that different types of sport require different perspectives.
In 1985, Paivio developed an analytic framework that has received critical acclaim. It provides an accurate way of viewing the process of imagery. Paivio states that imagery involves rehearsal of specific skills (cognitive specific - CS) and strategies of play (cognitive general - CG). In addition, motivational function, at specific level, involves imagining a goal and then imagining a course of action that results in the achievement of that goal (motivational specific - MS).
Sackett (1934) also investigated the nature and benefits of mental imagery – developing his ‘symbolic learning theory’. In his work, Sackett states that imagery operates as a cognitive coding system. In terms of sport, this means that athletes can obtain ‘mental blue-prints’ of a given motor pattern. Imaging strengthens these blue prints (Hall et al, 1990), providing further benefits for athletic performance.
Hall et al identified 2 sub-sections of MG, these being:
- MGa – motivational general arousal (to do with arousal and stress)
- MGm – motivational general mastery (control, toughness,confidence)
Hall et al developed SIQ (Sport Imagery Questionnaire) to measure the extent at which the 5 types – CS, CG, MS, MGa, MGm - of imaging are used.
The SIQ was used in this study, with the intention of identifying the use of imagery.
The hypotheses are:
Experimental - There will be a significant difference in imagery used across each subscale of the SIQ between the recreational and competitive groups.
Null - There will not be a significant difference in imagery use across each subscale of the SIQ between the recreational and competitive groups.
METHOD
Participants
There were 54 sports studies students, aged between 19 and 23, were asked to complete the SIQ (Sports Imagery Questionnaire). 18 (9 male, 9 female) of these played sport at recreational level and 36 (18 male, 18 female) at competitive level – this being defined as competing above school level. All participants were asked if they wanted to take part and all agreed. No controls were instated on age, gender, occupation or fitness levels.
Design
The results taken from the sample were recorded on a questionnaire basis, as the ‘Sport Imagery Questionnaire’ (SIQ) was adopted. The questionnaire consisted of 30 questions to be completed via self report with the participants assessing their own level of imagery contribution to the given situations on a scale from 1 to 7 for each of the 30 questions, 1 meaning rarely occurring up to 7 which means occurring often. The 30 questions on the SIQ were used to total up scores for each of the 5 subscales of imagery theory as described in the introduction.
Independent T-tests were used to analyse the data gained from the questionnaires.
Materials
Copy of the SIQ (Sports Imagery Questionnaire)
Answer sheets for the SIQ
Procedure
The participants were each told that all scores would be anonymous and confidential and that there was no obligation to fill in the questionnaire. The participants were also told that they could withdraw at any time.
The participants received a copy of the Sports Imagery Questionnaire (SIQ) and filled in the cover sheet – declaring their age, sex, sport they competed in and their level of competition (‘competitive’ being above school level). They then had to answer the questions using the Likert Scales (1-7, 1 being ‘rarely’ and 7 being ‘often’).
After the SIQ was completed the SIQ score sheet was used to obtain mean scores for the 5 different categories of imagery. These 5 imagery headings are:
- CS – Cognitive specific
- CG – Cognitive general
- MS – Motivational specific
- MG-A – Motivational general-arousal
- MG-M – Motivational general-mastery
The SIQ score sheet was then used in conjunction with the SIQ test to generate mean scores for the five different categories of imagery. Therefore each participant contributed a mean score under each of the five different imagery headings: CS, CG, MS, MGM, MGA. This data was then analysed and an internal consistency figure of 0.8841(Cronbach’s alpha) was used to test the reliability of the questionnaire.
RESULTS
The above chart show the mean values for the SIQ, these can be found in the table below.
- CS – recreational
- CS – competitive
- CG - recreational
- CG - competitive
- MS - recreational
- MS - competitive
- MGM - recreational
- MGM - competitive
- MGA - recreational
- MGA – competitive
Table 1
In the above table the abbreviations for the imagery subscale are explained earlier in the procedure section. Under the heading Level R stands for recreational and C for competitive participants.
Table 2
In the above table Df stands for degrees of freedom. Sig. stands for significant where significant is p>0.05
DISCUSSION
The results from table 2 i.e. t-test for equality of means show that the results for Cognitive general 0.005, Motivational specific 0.04 and Motivational general-mastery 0.04 are all significant results as p>0.05. This means that for these 3 subscales of imagery there is support for the experimental hypothesis in that there was a significant difference in the results obtained from the SIQ by the recreational and competitive participants. Referring back to the introduction and firstly the work by Mahoney and Avener(1977) into the use of imagery in gymnasts. The findings that the elite gymnasts who qualified for the Olympic team favoured the internal perspective of imagery as opposed to the external perspective. This could also explain why the Cognitive general, Motivational specific and the Motivational general-mastery subscales were found to be significant. Cognitive general is the performer imagining the plan they have ready being executed successfully, Motivational specific is where the performer imagines specific goals and Motivational general-mastery incorporates such imagery as mastering the competitive situation, the notion of being in control. Internal imagery is through the eyes of the athlete – in a first person view, this would make it possible for a competitor of a higher level to see his/her mistakes from the three above mentioned subscales, then commit these mistakes to memory so as to be avoided in the future. This type of mental practice would be of more aid to the participant than the external imagery perspective, which is an outside view of performance.
This does not necessarily mean that recreational participants use only the external perspective, only that with increased calibre of competition, the participants are more skilled and so get more from the mental imagery practice.
In conclusion, athletes use imagery more at competition stage then at practice. This is because imagery is used to help keep an athlete focused on their event and in control of their arousal levels and emotions, which is why imagery is used just prior to competitions. The main goal of this study – to compare the use of imagery in competitive and recreational sport, was in part achieved with three of the subscales outlined in Paivios conceptual framework being shown to have significant differences for recreational and competitive participants. However, no controls were instated on gender, age or type of sport. Also, no distinction was made as to whether the participants were involved in team or individual sports, as it could be proved in further research that those partaking in team sports might just as easily use the external imagery perspective to improve technical skill as a team.
References
Feltz, D. & Landers, D.(1983) The Effects of Mental Practice on Motor Skill Learning and Performance: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sport Psychology. 5, 25-57.
Gammage et al. (2000) More about exercise psychology. Sport Psychologist, 14, 348-359.
Hall, C. R. Rodgers, W. M. & Barr, K. A.(1990) The Use of Imagery by Athletes in Selected Sports. The Sport Psychologist. 4, 1-10. Human Kinetics Publishers, Inc.
Hardy, L & Callow, N. (1999) Efficacy of external and internal visual imagery perspectives for the enhancement of performance on tasks in which forms is important. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 21, 95-122.
Mahoney, M.J. & Avener, M (1977) Psychology of the Elite Athlete: An Explanatory Study. Cognitive Therapy and Research. 1, 135-141.
Murphy, A.M (1994) Imagery Interventions in Sport. Medicine and Science in Sports and Exercise. 26, 486-494.
Paivio. A (1985) Cognitive and Motivational Functions of Imagery in Human Performance. Canadian journal of Applied Sports Science. 10, 22-28.
Rotella, R. J. Gansneder, B. Ojala, D. & Billing, J. (1980) Cognitions and Coping Strategies of Elite Skiers. An Explanatory Study of Young Developing Athletes. Journal of Sport Psychology. 2, 350-354.
White, A & Hardy, L. (1995) Use of different imagery perspectives on the learning and performance of different motor skills. British Journal of Psychology, 86, 169-180.
Comparing The Use of Imagery in Recreational
And Competitive Sport
Candidate Name: David Millington
Student Number: 20075574
Assignment Title: Assignment 2, Lab Report
Course Code: SPO2004