Emotions and facial processing of emotions

Authors Avatar by rrh123 (student)

“A frown is a smile upside down” – Fats Waller

Emotions have played an important role in our evolution and the ability to express these come from environmental cues (Darwin, 1872).  In 1999, Ekman provided criteria for emotions (see table 1). He and Friesen (1975) also identified six emotions (happiness, disgust, surprise, sadness, anger and fear) that were universal, that is these emotions were represented facially the same way across different cultures of the world. As a result, these six facial expressions tend to be termed as innate and hence support the “nurture” side of the debate in the famous “Nature vs Nurture” theme in Psychology.

Amongst these emotions, of key interest to psychologists are the emotions of anger and happiness. Hansen and Hansen (1988) concluded that it was easier for participants to detect an angry face within a set of happy faces. This is generally called the anger superiority effect. Further evidence (amongst others) for this effect was given by Fox, Lester, Russo, Bowles, Pichler, and Dutton (2000). Contrastingly, a research conducted by Juth, Lundqvist, Karlsson and Ohman (2005) found out that it was the facial expression of happiness that was detected faster than other emotions. This occurrence is termed as the happiness superiority effect. Conclusively, there is evidence (through                                                          empirical studies) that some emotions expressed in the face                                                       can be detected faster than others, namely anger and happiness.  Table 1-Criteria for “basic emotions” Ekman (1999) 

When studying facial emotions and its effect on attention, psychologists use reaction time(s) (RT) and accuracy as measures of performance, the dependent variables. A typical experimental set up comprises of participants who have to look for a target stimulus amongst many other distractors. A target stimulus is the one which the experimenter wants the participant to look for/identify. A distractor (in this case) is a stimulus that limits attention from the target stimulus. In Hansen and Hansen’s (1988) experiment, the target stimuli were the two facial expressions of anger and happiness. The third target stimulus was a neutral face. These target stimuli are also known as “discrepant faces”. Each one of these discrepant faces was shown with the other two corresponding stimuli respectively that acted as                                                                                                                                          distractors (see Figure 1). The participants were told to detect the discrepant face

and their RT as well as accuracy

         Figure 1-Diagramatic representation of Hansen and Hansen’s (1988) experimental set-up on discrepant faces              was measured.  To summarize the results of their first experiment, Hansen and Hansen found that it took a shorter time for participants to detect angry faces when they were presented amongst happy or neutral distractors. The results also showed that it took a longer time to detect happy faces from angry ones, on neutral distractors.

Join now!

In their second experiment, Hansen and Hansen used four faces, each one having a discrepant face. There were two situations to the experiment – either an angry face in a happy crowd or vice versa. Identification of where exactly the discrepant face was located was the key to this experiment. Again, the results were consistent with the previous experiment that it was easier to detect an angry discrepant face in a happy crowd than detecting a happy face in an angry crowd. A third experiment was also conducted on the anger superiority effect by Hansen and Hansen. This experimental set ...

This is a preview of the whole essay